Bigger is not always better: hypothesis testing in sexual evolution

Bigger is not always better: hypothesis testing in sexual evolution

Sexual selection can lead to fascinating phenomena. We are all familiar with the fabulous color display of male peacocks to attract females. Less well known, but definitely not less interesting, are stalk-eyed flies.

Posted by

Olga Krips

Published on

Thu 20 Aug. 2015

Sexual selection can lead to fascinating phenomena. We are all familiar with the fabulous color display of male peacocks to attract females. Less well known, but definitely not less interesting, are stalk-eyed flies. Due to the fact that the females strongly prefer males with wideset eyes, the males have developed eyes on stalks that can be larger than their bodies. And did you know that the Irish elk developed antlers through sexual selection that span over two-and-a-half meters? Some people believe that the males with antlers this large could hardly move through the forest, which may have led to the species’ extinction. It’s no wonder that sexual preference is so well-studied with so many hypotheses formulated in relation to it.

Adaptive canalization hypothesis

The adaptive canalization hypothesis is one of these evolutionary hypotheses on sexual preference. In simple words, it says that a feature that is important for fitness will have little variation. For example, if it is important for reproductive success to have a red nose, natural selection will lead to a high proportion of red noses in the population. If the color of the nose is not important, more individuals will have noses with other colors.

Validating the hypothesis 

Andreas Walzer and Peter Schausberger [1] studied the validity of the adaptive canalization hypothesis with two species of predatory mites. Predatory mites are tiny spider-like animals that eat plant-eating pest species and are, as a result, commonly used as biological control agents. The two species used were Phytoseiulus persimilis and Neoseiulus californicus. Phytoseiulus persimilis males vary little in body size and will, for readability, be called LV (low variation). Neoseiulus californicus males vary much more in size and will be called HV (high variation). The authors investigated the mating behavior of the mites and automated the study with EthoVision XT video tracking software.








FREE TRIAL: Try EthoVision XT yourself!

Request a free trial and find out what EthoVision XT can do for your research!

  • A cost-effective solution
  • Powerful data selection
  • Most cited video tracking system

To be picky or not to be picky

If the adaptive canalization hypothesis is true, there is sexual selection on body size in LV animals and not, or less-noticeably, in HV animals. Therefore, LV females should prefer larger males, which are assumed to be more fit than smaller ones. On the other hand, HV females should be less picky about male body size. This is exactly what the authors found. LV females spent more time with larger males, mated more often with them, and for longer periods of time than with small males. However, male size did not matter to HV females, which is also in agreement with the proposed theory.

Waterloo revisited

Interestingly, the authors also discovered that small HV males were more aggressive than the larger ones. It is possible that they needed to be aggressive to compete with larger males for mating. This phenomenon leads us to another interesting theory in sexual evolution, the Napoleon complex theory, which states that small men are aggressive to compensate for their lack of height. But that is a completely different story which may be a subject for a future blog.

Reference

Walzer, A., & Schausberger, P. (2015). Interdependent effects of male and female body size plasticity on mating behaviour of predatory mites. Animal behaviour, 100, 96-105.

Related Posts

Video tracking makes bird watching much easier
01 Jun animal behavior research Other (Animal)

Video tracking makes bird watching much easier

Everyone with children knows the phenomenon that what is good for a child is not always the best for a parent. The same counts for birds.
Network and learn – our favorite neuroscience conferences in 2023
27 Dec animal behavior research Other (Animal)

Network and learn – our favorite neuroscience conferences in 2023

We’ve put together a short list of the best (behavioral) neuroscience conferences for you to attend. Opportunities to meet up and talk science!
Picky cats and tasty food – sniffing is an indicator for tastiness
20 Nov animal behavior research Other (Animal)

Picky cats and tasty food – sniffing is an indicator for tastiness

Cats can be extremely picky when it comes to food. If the cat doesn’t like it, it will refuse to eat. Reason enough for the pet food industry to try to find out what cats really like.